
Dyslexia is a learning difference that affects a person’s ability to read accurately, fluently, and with comprehension. It occurs in people across all cultures and languages and is not a reflection of intelligence or motivation.
In recent years, advancements in neuroscience, genetics, education, and technology have significantly deepened our understanding of this complex and multifaceted condition. This article outlines 40 updated dyslexia facts, organized into seven key categories, designed to inform educators, parents, clinicians, and advocates while empowering individuals with dyslexia themselves.
We specialize in helping children overcome the symptoms of dyslexia. Book a free consultation to discuss your child’s learning needs.
Dyslexia facts #1-9: What dyslexia is (and isn’t)
- Dyslexia is said to be a neurodevelopmental difference, meaning it arises from how the brain develops and processes language—especially written language.
. - It is not a visual problem. Dyslexia does not stem from having poor eyesight.
. - Dyslexia can affect anyone, regardless of IQ. Reading struggles are seen in individuals across the intelligence spectrum, debunking the “bright but struggling” stereotype.
. - It typically involves weaknesses in phonological processing, although visual processing difficulties may also be present.
. - Decoding difficulty is a core symptom—individuals struggle to sound out unfamiliar words and apply letter-sound rules.
. - Spelling is often severely affected due to weaknesses in phonological memory, sound-symbol mapping, and sequencing.
. - Dyslexia is not a developmental delay that children outgrow. Without intervention, difficulties persist into adolescence and adulthood.
. - Letter reversals are not diagnostic. Reversing letters (like b/d) is common in early writing, even for children without dyslexia.
. - Dyslexia is not caused by poor teaching or lack of exposure to books. It occurs across all backgrounds and levels of instruction.
Dyslexia facts #10-15: Prevalence and demographics

- Dyslexia affects approximately 5–15% of the global population, though numbers may vary depending on diagnostic criteria.
. - Languages with more transparent spelling systems (like Finnish or Italian) show lower prevalence rates than opaque languages like English.
. - Boys are diagnosed 2–3 times more often than girls, although this may reflect behavioral biases in referrals rather than true prevalence.
. - Dyslexia often co-occurs with ADHD, with overlapping challenges in attention and executive function.
. - Diagnosis and support access are strongly influenced by socioeconomic status and geographic region.
. - Better awareness, screening, and policy shifts have led to rising global diagnosis rates, reducing stigma in many communities.
Dyslexia facts #16-22: Diagnosis and neurobiology
- Brain imaging consistently shows that people with dyslexia have reduced activity in two key areas when reading: the left temporo-parietal cortex, which helps link sounds to letters, and the left occipitotemporal region, including the Visual Word Form Area (VWFA), which is critical for recognizing written words quickly and effortlessly.
. - Researchers found that no single brain region could predict dyslexia. Instead, they discovered that children’s brains are organized around “hubs” — like a well-connected train network or social media platform. Children with efficient brain hubs had either mild or very specific difficulties, such as poor listening skills. But those with poorly connected hubs had widespread and severe problems — including dyslexia. This suggests that dyslexia may result from weak communication across brain systems rather than a defect in any one spot.
. - While phonological processing problems are common in dyslexia, recent research supports a Multiple Deficit Model (MDM) that explains reading difficulties as arising from a combination of cognitive, genetic, and environmental risk factors. These can include weaknesses in attention, processing speed, working memory, or even orthographic processing, in addition to phonology.
. - Neuroimaging studies have shown that the Visual Word Form Area (VWFA) becomes specialized for distinguishing mirror-image letters like b and d. In dyslexia, this area is underactivated and lacks mirror-sensitivity, making persistent letter reversals a neurological reality — not just a developmental phase.
. - Eye-tracking research has shown that dyslexic readers often exhibit unstable eye movements (saccades), which may impact fluency.
. - Many children who struggle with reading (dyslexia) also struggle with math (dyscalculia), and research shows this is not just a coincidence. Brain imaging studies reveal shared patterns of underactivation in areas involved in processing symbols, sequences, and working memory.
. - A history of early language delay is one of the strongest developmental indicators of future reading challenges.
Dyslexia facts #23-27: Genetic and environmental factors

- Dyslexia has a strong genetic basis, with heritability estimates between 40% and 70%.
. - Studies of identical twins show higher concordance rates for dyslexia than fraternal twins, emphasizing heritability.
. - Prenatal exposure to substances like nicotine and alcohol is associated with a higher likelihood of developing dyslexia.
. - Parental reading behaviors and early language exposure can buffer or exacerbate genetic risks.
. - Dyslexia results from the complex interplay between multiple genes and environmental influences—there is no single “dyslexia gene.”
Dyslexia facts #28-32: Cognitive and linguistic features
- Delays in expressive language during early childhood can signal a higher risk for later reading difficulties.
. - Core deficits include challenges in phonological awareness, working memory, rapid automatized naming (RAN), and letter-sound correspondence.
. - Despite reading difficulties, many individuals with dyslexia possess strong oral comprehension, problem-solving, and reasoning skills.
. - Some research suggests strengths in visual-spatial reasoning and creativity, though the concept of a “dyslexic advantage” is still debated.
. - Students with dyslexia often find it hard to learn additional languages — especially when instruction focuses heavily on sounding out words (phonetic decoding), which relies on skills they already struggle with.
Dyslexia facts #33-37: Educational interventions
- Individualized, one-on-one tutoring generally yields better outcomes than group instruction.
. - Students with dyslexia can make significant gains even in upper elementary and secondary school with intensive support.
. - While Orton-Gillingham–based programs are widely used and structured in a way that supports dyslexic learners, high-quality studies have shown mixed results. In a 2021 meta-analysis, OG-based methods had only small effects.
. - The most effective dyslexia interventions go beyond just phonics and comprehension. They also build foundational cognitive skills — like phonemic awareness, working memory, attention, and sequencing — which are essential for long-term reading success.
. - Despite being popular, colored overlays do not meaningfully improve reading for most dyslexic students.
Dyslexia facts #38-40: Psychosocial and emotional aspects
- Children with dyslexia face increased risks for anxiety, low self-esteem, and depression, especially when unsupported.
. - Positive relationships with teachers, peers, and family, as well as success in areas like arts or sports, build resilience.
. - Advocacy movements and social media campaigns have significantly reduced stigma and increased global visibility for people with dyslexia.
Conclusion
Dyslexia is not a reflection of intelligence, laziness, or poor teaching — it is a complex, multifactorial difference in how the brain processes written language. Over the past five years, research from neuroscience, genetics, psychology, and education has shifted our understanding from narrow phonics-based models to broader, more nuanced frameworks. These include the Multiple Deficit Model, network-based brain theories, and an emphasis on early relational environments that shape language and reading development from birth.
At the same time, technology and teaching practices have evolved. We now know that structured, individualized instruction can drive significant improvement — even in older students — and that fostering self-belief, resilience, and supportive relationships is just as critical as teaching decoding skills.
Dyslexia is not a fixed identity. It’s a learning difference that exists on a spectrum, shaped by genes, environment, and opportunity. With early recognition, high-quality intervention, and emotionally safe instruction, students with dyslexia can not only learn to read — they can thrive.
This article aimed to bring together the most up-to-date findings in one accessible place. Whether you are an educator, a parent, a policymaker, or someone with dyslexia yourself, the message is clear:
Dyslexia is real; it is complex, but with the right support, it is surmountable.
Key takeaways from 40 Up-to-Date Dyslexia Facts:
- Dyslexia is a neurodevelopmental language difference, not a vision problem or a sign of low intelligence. It often involves difficulties with phonological processing, memory, and sequencing.
. - Brain scans show underactivity and poor connectivity in reading networks — especially in the left hemisphere. Dyslexia is not caused by a single brain defect but by weak coordination across systems.
. - The Multiple Deficit Model now dominates research, showing that dyslexia results from a mix of genetic, cognitive, and environmental factors — not just phonics weaknesses.
. - Early expressive language delays, family history, and co-occurring challenges (like ADHD or dyscalculia) are strong indicators of future reading difficulties.
. - Effective interventions go beyond phonics. They also support working memory, attention, and language systems — especially when delivered one-on-one and intensively.
. - Dyslexia symptoms can be overcome. Students can build confidence and thrive academically and beyond with structured instruction and emotional support.
References for 40 Up-to-Date Dyslexia Facts:
- Bishop, D. V. M. (2015). The interface between genetics and psychology: Lessons from developmental dyslexia. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 282.
- Brem, S., Maurer, U., Kronbichler, M. et al. (2020). Visual word form processing deficits driven by severity of reading impairments in children with developmental dyslexia. Scientific Reports, 10.
- Elliott, J. G., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2014). The dyslexia debate. Cambridge University Press.
- Elliott, J. G., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2024). The dyslexia debate revisited. Cambridge University Press.
- Pedago, F., Nakamura, K., Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2011). Breaking the symmetry: Mirror discrimination for single letters but not for pictures in the Visual Word Form Area. Neuroimage, 55: 742-9.
- Pennington, B. F. (2006). From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders. Cognition, 101(2), 385-413.
- Peterson, R. L., & Pennington, B. F. (2012). Developmental dyslexia. The Lancet, 379(9830), 1997–2007.
- Shaywitz, S. (2003). Overcoming dyslexia. New York, NY: Alfred A. Knopf.
- Siugzdaite, R., Bathelt, J., Holmes, J., & Astle, D. E. (2020). Transdiagnostic brain mapping in developmental disorders. Current Biology, 30(7).
- Snowling, M. J., & Hulme, C. (2021). Annual Research Review: Reading disorders revisited – the critical importance of oral language. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 62(5), 635–644.
- Stevens, E. A., Austin, C., Moore, C., Scammacca, N., Boucher, A. N., & Vaughn, S. (2021). Current state of the evidence: Examining the effects of Orton-Gillingham reading interventions for students with or at risk for word-level reading disabilities. Exceptional Children, 87(4), 397–417.
- Tanaka, H., Black, J. M., Hulme, C., Stanley, L. M., Kesler, S. R., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., … Hoeft, F. (2011). The brain basis of the phonological deficit in dyslexia is independent of IQ. Psychological Science, 22(11), 1442-1451.
- Uccula, A., Enna, M., & Mulatti, C. (2014). Colors, colored overlays, and reading skills. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 833.
- Werth, R. (2021). Is developmental dyslexia due to a visual and not a phonological impairment? Brain Sciences, 11(10), 1313.
- Wilkins, A. J. (2002). Coloured overlays and their effects on reading speed: A review. Journal of Research in Reading, 25(1), 54–68.
- Wood, T. (2011). Overcoming dyslexia for dummies. Indianapolis: Wiley Publishing, Inc.
Authored by Sue du Plessis (B.A. Hons Psychology; B.D.), an educational and reading specialist with 30+ years of experience in learning disabilities.